Close Window
Aetna Aetna
Clinical Policy Bulletin:
Visual Perceptual Training and Vision Restoration Therapy
Number: 0321


Policy

Aetna considers visual perceptual training experimental and investigational for the treatment of perceptual dysfunctions and for all other indications because its effectiveness has not been validated in well-designed prospective clinical studies.

Aetna considers vision restoration therapy experimental and investigational for the treatment of visual field deficits due to ischemic optic neuropathy, neurotrauma, or stroke, and for all other indications because its effectiveness has not been validated in well-designed prospective clinical studies.

Note: Visual perceptual training and vision restoration therapy should be distinguished from optometric vision therapy.  See CPB 0489 - Vision Therapy.



Background

Visual perceptual training is a psycho-educational intervention that focuses on perceptual dysfunctions that are claimed to contribute to delay in speech and language development in preschool children.  The Handbook of Visual Perceptual Training (Cunningham and Reagan, 1972) (the Handbook) defines visual perception as "that process by which impressions observed through the medium of the eye are transmitted to the brain where relationship to past experiences takes place."

According to the Handbook, "it is concluded that visual perceptual deficits fall into patterns of a syndrome and that each component may impinge upon any number of other factors or may function independently.  Visual perceptual dysfunction does not include lack of visual perceptual stimulation; it does involve improper choice of ontogenetic sequencing for such stimulation.  It is not a matter of either-or; rather it is a matter of degree.  It represents an inefficient developmental functioning that is a handicap to cognitive process.  It is related to both cognition and emotional development" (Cunningham and Reagan, 1972).  The authors of the Handbook further note that "concomitant factors of visual perceptual dysfunction may be short attention span, hyperactivity, distractibility, social adjustment difficulties, delayed motor perceptual ability, depressed academic achievement, inadequate body image and low frustration level."  "Visual perception dysfunction," according to the Handbook, "is to be classified as a learning disability and language disorder."

Visual perception training programs involve an "integrated program involving speech and language activities, a wide range of sensory modalities and visual-motor perceptual activities" (Cunningham and Reagan, 1972).  These activities include motor rhythm activities, body image training, as well as training in spatial and directional relationships.  "Suggested activities are grouped under five main headings: coordination of eye-motor movements, distinguishing foreground from background, visual memory, spatial position and relationship to space ... Included in the activities are speech, language and visual-motor perceptual tasks that involve use of all senses."

Although vision perception training may include some exercises similar to vision therapy exercises, visual perceptual training should be distinguished from optometric vision therapy.  Visual perceptual training is directed toward perceptual dysfunctions that allegedly affect language and learning abilities, whereas vision therapy is a set of exercises directed toward specific deficiencies in the movements and/or focusing of the eye (e.g., convergence insufficiency, disorders of accommodation, esophoria, strabismus, etc.).  Patients receive vision therapy to treat visual disturbances that may theoretically cause developmental delays and learning disabilities, whereas patients may receive vision perception training to remedy developmental delays and learning disabilities without having any identified dysfunction of eye movements or focusing.

Patients receive vision therapy from eye care professionals, whereas visual perceptual training is generally performed by psychologists, psychotherapists, and other behavioral health professionals.

A position statement by the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP), the American Academy of Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus (AAOPOS), and the American Academy of Ophthalmology (1998) concluded that there is insufficient scientific evidence to support claims that academic abilities of children with learning disabilities can be improved with visual perceptual training.

Vision restoration therapy (VRT) targets the vision center of the brain and is intended to improve visual function in patients with visual field deficits that may result from brain injury or stroke.  Patients utilize a computer screen to focus on a displayed central point and respond every time they see light stimuli appear.  The light stimuli are presented in the area most likely to recover visual function, an area that will change as therapy progresses and vision is improved.  While there are studies evaluating the usefulness of VRT, there is inadequate evidence of effectiveness for this treatment.

Meuller et al (2003) performed a retrospective analysis of 69 patients with visual field deficits following neurotrauma or stroke after they had performed a 6 month regimen of VRT.  Specifically, these researchers wanted to ascertain (i) if VRT affects activities of daily life (ADL) measures, and (ii) to what extent any subjective changes correlate with quantitative measures of visual field enlargements.  A retrospective analysis was performed with data of 69 patients who had been interviewed after 6 months of VRT.  Patient testimonials were analyzed post-hoc and correlated with demographic status as well as pre- and post-RT changes as measured by perimetric testing.  As previously described, VRT significantly increased detection ability and most patients (88 %) reported subjective benefits in ADL.  A correlation analysis of quantitative parameters of visual field enlargements with subjective patient testimonials was performed.  Significant correlation was found in the categories "carrying out hobbies" (r = 0.360) and for "general improvement of vision" (r = 0.244).  A trend was evident for the category "reading" (r = 0.215).  No correlation was found between visual field size improvements and "visual confidence/mobility" and "ability to avoid collisions".  Thus, visual field size appears only to be one, surprisingly minor, factor among others (such as temporal processing) determining subjective vision in brain damaged patients.

Meuller et al (2007) evaluated the outcome of VRT in a large sample of clinical patients and studied factors contributing to subjective and objective measures of visual field alterations.  Clinical observational analysis of visual fields was performed in 302 patients before and after being treated with computer-based VRT for a period of 6 months at 8 clinical centers in central Europe.  The visual field defects were due to ischemia, hemorrhage, head trauma, tumor removal or anterior ischemic optic neuropathy.  Primary outcome measure was a visual field assessment with super-threshold perimetry.  Additionally, conventional near-threshold perimetry, eye movements and subjective reports of daily life activities were assessed in a subset of the patients.  Vision restoration therapy improved patients' ability to detect super-threshold stimuli in the previously deficient area of the visual field by 17.2 % and these detection gains were not significantly correlated with eye movements.  Notable improvements were seen in 70.9 % of the patients.  Efficacy was independent of lesion age and etiology, but patients with larger areas of residual vision at baseline and patients over 65-year of age benefited most.  Conventional perimetry validated visual field enlargements and patient testimonials confirmed the improvement in every day visual functions.  The authors concluded that VRT improves visual functions in a large clinical sample of patients with visual field defects involving the CNS, confirming former experimental studies.  The lack of a control group limited the validity of the findings of this study.

In a retrospective study, Romano and colleagues (2008) examined the effect of a visual rehabilitation intervention on visual field defects in a U.S. cohort.  Vision restoration therapy consists of a specific pattern of stimulation that is directed at the border of the blind field.  This study evaluated individuals with homonymous visual field defect from retrochiasmatic lesions treated with 6 modules of VRT.  Supra-threshold visual field testing of the central 43 x 32 was obtained at baseline and after each module.  The main outcome measures were the change in stimuli detection and the shift in the position of the border of the blind field.  The impact of age, time from injury and type of visual field defect were analyzed.  Among 161 patients, the mean absolute improvement in stimuli detection was 12.8 %.  The average border shift was 4.87.  Improvements of greater than or equal to 3 % was noted in 76 % of patients.  Absolute change in stimulus detection of greater than or equal to % at mid-therapy was associated with a greater final improvement.  Age, time from lesion and type of visual field defect did not influence the degree of field expansion.  The authors concluded that VRT improves stimulus detection and results in a shift of the position of the border of the blind field as measured on suprathreshold visual field testing.  These results support prior reports and support VRT as a useful rehabilitative intervention for a proportion of patients with visual field defects from retrochiasmatic lesions.  However, the findings of this study were limited by lack of (i) randomization, (ii) control group and (iii) long-term follow-up.

In a pilot study, Jung et al (2008) evaluated the effects of VRT on the visual function of 10 patients with stable anterior ischemic optic neuropathy (AION).  All patients were evaluated before VRT and after 3 and 6 months of treatment by Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, reading speed, 24-2 SITA-standard Humphrey visual field (HVF), high-resolution perimetry (HRP) (perimetry obtained during VRT), and vision-based quality of life questionnaire.  Patients were randomized between 2 VRT strategies (5 in each group): (i) VRT in which stimulation was performed in the seeing VF of the affected eye ("seeing field-VRT"); and (ii) VRT in which stimulation was performed along the area of central fixation and in the ARV (areas of residual vision) of the affected eye ("ARV-VRT").  The results of the HRP, HVF, and clinical assessment of visual function were compared for each patient and between the 2 groups at each evaluation.  Visual acuity qualitatively improved in the ARV-VRT group, however the change was not statistically significant (p = 0.28).  Binocular reading speed significantly improved in the ARV-VRT group (p = 0.03).  HVF foveal sensitivity increased mildly in both groups (p = 0.059); HRP analysis showed a similar increase in stimulus accuracy in both groups (mean improvement of about 15 %).  All patients reported functional improvement after VRT.  The authors concluded that despite a small sample, the study showed a trend toward improvement of visual function in the ARV-VRT group.  Improvement of HRP in both groups may reflect diffusely increased visual attention (neuronal activation), or improvement of an underlying sub-clinical abnormality in the "seeing" visual field of patients with optic neuropathies.  The author noted that a small sample size limited the conclusions that can be reached from this study.

Glisson (2006) noted that VRT has shown promise as a treatment strategy to improve visual field deficits in patients with lesions of the brain or optic nerve.  However, objective measures of its effectiveness have remained controversial.  The author reviewed the current theories supporting the reported benefits of VRT, and the dissenting opinions, reconsidered VRT as an emerging therapy.  The benefits of VRT have been challenged by a study suggesting that no improvement exists with careful control of fixation.  Alternatively, others suggested that eye movements are not induced by VRT.  Functional imaging studies demonstrated the potential role of plasticity in VRT.  While the exact mechanism remains to be elucidated, subjective improvement in daily functioning iwa reported in a significant percentage of patients.  The author concluded that VRT is a non-invasive, home-based strategy for the rehabilitation of patients with visual field loss caused by structural or ischemic damage.  While subjective benefits in functional status have been reported by patients following completion of the program, debate centered around the inadequacy of the methods used to document its effectiveness.  Until such a method is validated by carefully controlled studies, subjective improvement in visual function stands alone as evidence of VRT's benefit.

McFadzean (2006) reviewed the controversial findings for NovaVision's VRT.  It has been claimed that NovaVision's computerized therapy results in expansion of the visual field in optic nerve and occipital lesions, but the outcome has been challenged on the grounds of unsatisfactory perimetric control of central fixation and disputed mechanisms.  The author noted that in clinical practice NovaVision's VRT should not currently gain acceptance in view of unacceptable perimetric standards and equivocal results.  Possible effects on a relative scotoma at the edge of a lesion have not been adequately explored.  In the interim, research should also be focused on compensatory eye movement strategies.

Astle et al (2011) noted that amblyopia presents early in childhood and affects approximately 3 % of western populations.  The monocular visual acuity loss is conventionally treated during the “critical periods” of visual development by occluding or penalizing the fellow eye to encourage use of the amblyopic eye.  Despite the measurable success of this approach in many children, substantial numbers of people still suffer with amblyopia later in life because either they were never diagnosed in childhood, did not respond to the original treatment, the amblyopia was only partially remediated, or their acuity loss returned after cessation of treatment.  In this review, these researchers examined if the visual deficits of this largely over-looked amblyopic group are amenable to conventional and innovative therapeutic interventions later in life, well beyond the age at which treatment is thought to be effective.  There is a considerable body of evidence that residual plasticity is present in the adult visual brain and this can be harnessed to improve function in adults with amblyopia.  Perceptual training protocols have been developed to optimize visual gains in this clinical population.  Results thus far are extremely encouraging; marked visual improvements have been demonstrated, the perceptual benefits transfer to new visual tasks and appear to be relatively enduring.  The essential ingredients of perceptual training protocols are being incorporated into video game formats, facilitating home-based interventions.  The authors concluded that many studies support perceptual training as a tool for improving vision in amblyopes beyond the critical period.  They stated that should this novel form of treatment stand up to the scrutiny of a randomized controlled trial, clinicians may need to re-evaluate their therapeutic approach to adults with amblyopia.

Schinzel et al (2012) noted that there is a pilot study that examined if residual visual deficits after past or recent optic neuritis can be reduced by means of VRT.  They stated that if VRT is shown to improve visual function after optic neuritis, this method might be a first therapeutic option for patients with incomplete recovery from optic neuritis.

 
CPT Codes / HCPCS Codes / ICD-9 Codes
There is no specific CPT code for visual perceptual training:
There is no specific CPT code for visual restoration therapy:
Other CPT codes related to the CPB:
92065
97533
ICD-9 codes not covered for indications listed in the CPB (not all-inclusive):
315.31 - 315.39 Developmental speech or language disorder
368.40 - 368.47 Visual field defects
377.41 Ischemic optic neuropathy
433.00 - 434.91 Occlusion and stenosis of cerebral and precerebral arteries
783.42 Delayed milestones
784.40 - 784.5 Voice and speech disturbances
800 - 804.99 Fracture of skull [neurotrauma]
850.0 - 854.19 Intracranial injury
V40.1 Problems with communication [including speech]
V57.3 Speech therapy


The above policy is based on the following references:
  1. Cunningham SA, Reagan CL. Handbook of Visual Perceptual Training. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher; 1972.
  2. Carey JM. Therapeutic value of visual perceptual training. S Afr Med J. 1996;86(12):1561.
  3. Schoeman OJ. The therapeutic value of visual-perceptual training and its effect on scholastic achievement. S Afr Med J. 1996;86(8):983.
  4. Miller SR, Sabatino DA, Miller TL. Influence of training in visual perceptual discrimination on drawings by children. Percept Mot Skills. 1977;44(2):479-487.
  5. Bieger E. Effectiveness of visual perceptual training on reading skills of non-readers, an experimental study. Percept Mot Skills. 1974;38(3):1147-1153.
  6. Martin JC. Effects of visual perceptual training on visual perceptual skills and reading achievement. Percept Mot Skills. 1973;37(2):564.
  7. Buckland P, Balow B. Effect of visual perceptual training on reading achievement. Except Child. 1973;39(4):299-304.
  8. Walsh JF, D'Angelo R. Effectiveness of the Frostig program for visual perceptual training with Head Start children. Percept Mot Skills. 1971;32(3):944-946.
  9. Marks HB. Evaluation of visual perceptual training for reading disabilities. R I Med J. 1970;53(3):150-151 passim.
  10. Talkington LW. Frostig visual perceptual training with low-ability-level retarded. Percept Mot Skills. 1968;27(2):505-506.
  11. Alley GR. Perceptual-motor performances of mentally retarded children after systematic visual-perceptual training. Am J Ment Defic. 1968;73(2):247-250.
  12. Rosen CL. An experimental study of visual perceptual training and reading achievement in first grade. Percept Mot Skills. 1966;22(3):979-986.
  13. Grigorieva L, Bernadskaya M, Svechnikov V. Visual perceptual training of children with multiple disabilities in Russia. In: Proceedings of ICEVI's Xth World Conference. Stepping Forward Together: Families and Professionals as Partners in Achieving an Education for All. Sao Paulo, Brazil, August 3-8, 1997. L Campbell, M Campos, B Furry, R Mortimer, eds. Coimbatore, India: International Council on Education of People with Visual Impairment (ICEVI); 2000. Available at: http://www.icevi.org/. Accessed July 15, 2003.
  14. Hallahan DP, Mercer CD. Educational programming: Dominance of psychological processing and visual perceptual training. In: Learning Disabilities: Historical Perspectives. Learning Disabilities Summit: Building a Foundation for the Future White Papers. Nashville, TN: National Research Center for Learning Disabilities; August 2001. Available at: http://www.nrcld.org/html/information/articles/ldsummit/. Accessed May 10, 2005.
  15. American Academy of Pediatrics. Learning disabilities, dyslexia, and vision: A subject review. Committee on Children with Disabilities, American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) and American Academy of Ophthalmology (AAO), American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus (AAPOS). Pediatrics. 1998;102(5):1217-1219.
  16. De Wit L, Kamsteegt H, Yadav B, et al. Defining the content of individual physiotherapy and occupational therapy sessions for stroke patients in an inpatient rehabilitation setting. Development, validation and inter-rater reliability of a scoring list. Clin Rehabil. 2007;21(5):450-459.
  17. Si Hyun Kang, Kim DK, Kyung Mook Seo, et al. A computerized visual perception rehabilitation programme with interactive computer interface using motion tracking technology -- a randomized controlled, single-blinded, pilot clinical trial study. Clin Rehabil. 2009;23(5):434-444.
  18. Mueller I, Poggel DA, Kenkel S, et al. Vision restoration therapy after brain damage: Subjective improvements of activities of daily life and their relationship to visual field enlargements. Vis Impair Res. 2003;5(3):157-178.
  19. Glisson CC. Capturing the benefit of vision restoration therapy. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. 2006;17(6):504-508.
  20. McFadzean RM. NovaVision: Vision restoration therapy. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. 2006;17(6):498-503.
  21. Mueller I, Mast H, Sabel BA. Recovery of visual field defects: A large clinical observational study using vision restoration therapy. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2007;25(5-6):563-572.
  22. Romano J, Schulz P, Kenkel S, et al. Visual field changes after a rehabilitation intervention: Vision restoration therapy. J Neurol Sci. 2008;273(1-2):70-74.
  23. Jung CS, Bruce B, Newman NJ, Biousse V. Visual function in anterior ischemic optic neuropathy: Effect of Vision Restoration Therapy -- a pilot study. J Neurol Sci. 2008;268(1-2):145-149.
  24. Astle AT, Webb BS, McGraw PV. Can perceptual learning be used to treat amblyopia beyond the critical period of visual development? Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2011;31(6):564-573.
  25. Schinzel J, Schwarzlose L, Dietze H, et al. Efficacy of vision restoration therapy after optic neuritis (VISION study): Study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2012;13:94.


email this page   


Copyright Aetna Inc. All rights reserved. Clinical Policy Bulletins are developed by Aetna to assist in administering plan benefits and constitute neither offers of coverage nor medical advice. This Clinical Policy Bulletin contains only a partial, general description of plan or program benefits and does not constitute a contract. Aetna does not provide health care services and, therefore, cannot guarantee any results or outcomes. Participating providers are independent contractors in private practice and are neither employees nor agents of Aetna or its affiliates. Treating providers are solely responsible for medical advice and treatment of members. This Clinical Policy Bulletin may be updated and therefore is subject to change.
Aetna
Back to top